In this study, the investigator asks, "How are the flight patterns of
fireflies adapted for finding a mate?" The study compares the flight
patterns of two closely related fireflies: Photinus collustrans and Photinus tanytoxus, both of which are active in southern Florida from May to November.
In both species, males fly in search of flightless females, who are
responsive to the males during only a short period each evening. Both
species are found in relatively open habitats and may be found in the
same location, but they are distinguished by their activity at different
times of the evening.
P. collustrans begins its activity between 11 and 26 minutes
after sunset. Males flash once every 2.2 seconds at 71.6 degrees.
Females return a single flash 0.5 seconds after the male flash from
their location on the ground near their burrows. After engaging in this
activity for about 15 minutes, P. collustrans abruptly goes inactive.
Shortly after P. collustrans has ended its activity for the night, P. tanytoxus becomes active for 35 to 70 minutes. Its flash patterns are nearly identical to P. collustrans, but the primary difference is that the female response time is 1.5 seconds.
Since P. collustrans is active only for a 15 minute period
each evening, competition for females is intense. On any given night, a
male has only a 13% chance of finding a female. Therefore, male flight
patterns must be adapted to be as efficient as possible. And since
finding a mate is a two-way process (the male flashes and the female
responds) the male must fly in a a pattern that maximizes the chance
that a female will see him and respond.
It has been theorized that the most efficient search pattern would be
one of a straight line $#151; flying in a series of straight paths from
one side of the search area to the other. However, most animals do not
search in this way; their paths are influenced by outside factors such
as environmental features, the most likely habitat for females, etc. But
fireflies, which fly when many of these environmental features are
hidden by the dark, show a straighter search pattern than most animals.
Both P. collustrans and P. tanytoxus males show a
common pattern of change in their flight patterns throughout their
activity periods. In both cases, the males exhibit an increasingly
straight flight path as the night wears on. It is easy to understand why
this happens with P. collustrans, which begins its flight
shortly after sunset, when there is still light to see the environmental
features. As it becomes darker, these visual cues become less visible,
influencing the flight pattern less and resulting in a straighter path.
Given the influence of available light on flight direction, one would expect the late-emerging P. collustrans and early P. tanytoxus to have very similar flight patterns, since they fly at about the same time. However, the early P. tanytoxus have a less straight-line flight than the late P. collustrans. Clearly, something other than amount of available light is affecting their flight patterns.
One condition that is different for the late-emerging P. collustrans and early P. tanytoxus
is the availability of females. Since females are continually being
found and mated with during the activity period, there are fewer females
available at the end of the activity period. This means that late P. collustrans are searching for more thinly distributed females than early P. tanytoxus.
There is some evidence that suggests that the search patterns are
affected by the "expected" distribution of females. In areas with few
searching males, or during times late in the activity period with fewer
females responding to the males, the males tend to fly in a more direct
path to increase their chances of finding widely scattered isolated
females.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar